Do you know where my glasses are?
There’s the guy
who used to live across the street from us when we were in Philadelphia.
What was his name? Jack? Jim? What was it?
Can you button my
cuff for me?
Anyone could need
assistance similar to what is implied above. But such needs become more
frequent as we age. Are age-related declines inevitable? How deep
will they be?
Concerns about
ageing and competence are very common in 21st Century America where from 1950
to 2010, the age cohort 65 and over increased 213% whereas 15- to 64-year-olds
grew only 105%, and the under 15 population, only 45% (
http://www.pewglobal.org/),
Psychological
research provides standard textbook-type answers to age-related decline,
asserting, for instance, that "fluid" intelligence is most vulnerable
and "crystalized" intelligence is less. The former refers to capabilities that
require more abstraction, novelty, dexterity, and speed, and the latter, more
knowledge and experience, such as an extensive vocabulary, corpus of factual
details, or armamentarium of well-practiced strategies.
For good reasons,
age-related decline has been a frequent subject of scientific investigation.
Let’s consider one.
Nemanja Vaci,
Bartosz Gula, and Merim Bilalić (2015) investigated ageing though the lens of
chess competence by scrutinizing a database containing records of players from
beginner to world class. They
subdivided the course of chess competence into three phases: from start of
playing to peak performance, from peak to postpeak decline, and from post peak
decline to stabilization. At
first glance it was surprising to find that the more expert players’ declines
tended to occur earlier than did the declines of less capable ones. On the other hand, as anticipated, the
declines of the more expert ones slowed more quickly than did the declines of
the less capable ones. Not
surprisingly, regardless of expertise level, persons who played in the most
tournaments decline the slowest and the shallowest.
Am I smart enough
to learn how to play chess?
Am I willing to
put forth the time to become very skillful at it?
Am I good enough
to play in tournaments?
Am I able to
tolerate playing against tournament competitors who are very skillful?
The aforementioned
questions pertain to your self-confidence, priorities, time on task, determination,
and willingness to take risks. And all of those attributes will be critical for your learning, developing, maintaining competencies, and
reaching any goal. You certainly need self-confidence, priorities, time on task,
determination, and willingness to take risks to achieve a healthful lifestyle
no less than you do to become chess proficient.
And now, final implications from the chess study of Nemanja Vaci. I infer that the more expert players declined
earlier than less capable ones because the experts high level functioning
required much more fluid intelligence skills – the skills most sensitive to
ageing - than did their less capable counterparts. Relatedly, the decline of the former group of
chess players probably slowed more quickly because they had “overlearned” chess
(Overlearning is a process by which one practices a new skill with such focus
and frequency that the skill become habitual, requiring much less conscious effort
than otherwise warranted.) to the point that they retained considerable
chess-relevant fluid intellectual skills, eventually managing to compensate
well for the fluidity that they had lost.
And, of course, the expert and less capable chess players who played in
the most tournaments decline slowest and shallowest simply because the old adage of “use
it or lose it” undeniably is true.
Repeating my essential point then: To achieve a healthful
lifestyle, like an expert chess player, you must work continually toward
enhancing your self-confidence, priorities, time on task, determination, and
willingness to take health-promoting risks. Life is the ultimate chess game. Experts know the rules and strategies. And they know how to execute over the long term.
Reference
Vaci, N.,
Gula, B. &; Bilalić, M. (2015). Is age
really cruel to experts? Compensatory
effects of activity. Psychology and
Aging, 30, 4, 740-754. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pag0000056
No comments:
Post a Comment