Saturday, December 10, 2016

It’s Not So Crazy to Think That Sometime You Might Act Crazy


The human psyche, fortunately and necessarily, is oriented toward self-preservation.  If not, our species never would have survived.  Since we are the most physically dependent and the most social animal, our survival has demanded that we relate adaptively to those within our “tribe.”  Accordingly, humans developed extraordinary skill in understanding themselves and those around them.   Moreover, the two skills have been inextricably related—we understand ourselves by contrasting our behavior with that of our contemporaries and vice versa. 

Almost every day we observe someone doing something that we consider “crazy.”   Those crazy behaviors could include anything, from running away from home to never leaving the house.  Given our penchant for “social comparison,” we often imagine that we never could behave so maladaptively.  And, because of our “fundamental attribution error” predilection, we ascribe other people’s oddities to their enduring personalities while excusing ours as due to transient, external influences.  Moreover, to justify our perceptions about our odd neighbor, we can search through scores of mental illnesses enumerated in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM–5) to find one that seems just right.

Our weird neighbor is the exception of course.  There’s never anything strange about us.  After all, mental illness is rare.  Isn’t it?

The conventional view had been that mental illness was uncommon.  However, over the last decade several studies suggested otherwise.  For instance, the National Institute of Mental Health reported that 18 percent of American adults suffered from mental illness in 2014 (www.nimh.nih.gov), and Kessler et al. (2005) suggested that about 50 percent of us will evidence a diagnosable mental illness during our lifetime.     

If you think those statistics are ominous, consider the even more startling conclusions reached by Jonathan D. Schaefer and his colleagues (2015) who believed that previous reports most commonly employed three data collection methods that produced spurious low results.  First, national registries, they said, included mostly or exclusively persons who received treatment in psychiatric facilities, missing those treated in other settings or those not treated at all.  Second, retrospective studies primarily were limited to persons diagnosed with Axis I mental illnesses (e.g., Schizophrenia) and, therefore, missed Axis II and other serious problems (e.g., Psychopathic personality). The final inadequate data collection method employed prospective cohort studies.  Although prospective cohort studies (that follow persons of similar age over time) often are considered excellent, the problem for Schaefer was that the ones he uncovered also assessed only Axis I.  Despite that limitation, however, the prospective research did disclose a mental illness rate ranging from 61 to 85 percent, significantly higher than the other methods.

To remedy the perceived research inadequacies, the Schaefer group examined data from the Dunedin (New Zealand) cohort, a group that had been studied meticulously from their births until middle-age.  The entire cohort was scrutinized – not just those with diagnosed mental illness - and all diagnoses – not just Axis I - were considered.  Given that comprehensive, all-inclusive criterion, an astounding 83 percent of the study’s subjects suffered a diagnosable mental condition sometime during their lives.  Also surprising was that those who did escape all mental illness were not especially intelligent, physically healthy, or from a financially privileged family.  Rather, the emotionally sound 17 percent tended to be ones who had managed to maintain high-quality interpersonal relationships, to be more satisfied with their lives, and to have achieved greater educational and occupational success.


Before you conclude that we are doomed because almost everyone is going out of their minds, recall that Schaefer employed a very broad definition of mental illness.  There are so many mental diagnostic categories that virtually any imaginable problem can be labeled.  Moreover, the study did not adequately report  the severity or diagnostic distribution of the illnesses that were found.  There is a world of difference between an “Adjustment disorder with depressed mood” and “Schizophrenia, Paranoid Type.”  The Dunedin study can, in fact, be of comfort to you.  If emotional problems are so ubiquitous, then many, virtually by definition, are everyday problems of living.  So, when anxious, depressed or otherwise afflicted, remember that you probably are no more deviant than the weird neighbor mentioned earlier.  Do not let your emotions overwhelm you.  Keep moving forward, and as I implied in the subtitle of my Don’t Rest in Peace book, do your best to maintain an activity-oriented, physically and mentally integrated lifestyle.

References:

Kessler, R., et al. (2005)  Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM–IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication.  Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 593– 602. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/ archpsyc.62.6.593

Schaefer, J., et al. (2016)  Enduring mental health: Prevalence and prediction.  Journal of Abnormal Psychology, Dec, No Pagination Specified.  http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/abn0000232

No comments:

Post a Comment