You arrived very early to the party and have been eating and
drinking non-stop. Suddenly it hits you. Your stomach is
churning and your head is pounding. After wishing you were dead, you
resolve never to get yourself into that position of excess again. Two
weeks later you see a friend who also attended the party. She gushes
about how much she enjoyed each succulent shrimp and savory sip of
Scotch. You heartily agree, adding that the host is planning another
party on the 12th of the month, and that you too can’t wait to load up on the
food and drinks. Your previous experience of excess never enters your
mind. You have no intention of “depriving” yourself on the 12th.
How can a person go from resolving never to over-eat or over-drink
at a party to again behaving in just that way? Daniel Kahneman (2011)
would answer by differentiating the experiencing self from the remembering
self. These selves are as their names suggest. One experiences an
event in real time that she responds to in real time, and the other recalls the
experience later. The experience and the memory never are precisely the
same and, as in our example, the difference can be quite extreme.
The experiencing and remembering dichotomy has profound lifestyle
and health implications. It might lead one person continually to overeat
at parties and another continually to practice unsafe sex. Positive change
often begins with reconciling the experiencing and remembering selves.
That sounds easier than it is. Our memories are abstractions, often self-serving
abstractions that enable us to remember what we want and to forget the
rest. Because valid experiential feedback is essential for making
corrective lifestyle adjustments, we must learn how to remember enough of the
experiencing self impressions that we would prefer to forget.
Accurate memories start with a conscious intention to remember
accurately. But a global intention is not enough. Since memory is
built from abstracted impressions, we never will remember every single
detail. We typically remember what we already believe and/or what grabs
us emotionally. Therefore, we consciously must intend to remember the
specific aspects of our experiencing self that we would prefer to
dismiss. If that intention causes us to focus on the uncomfortable features
of our health-relevant experiences, we have a fighting chance to remember them
and to learn from them. And if our health-promoting intention and focus
permit us to begin to behave more healthfully, we have a fighting chance to
develop strong, enduring health habits.
To recall our specific health-relevant experiences we employ
"autobiographical episodic memory." And those memories have
been shown to provide content for about 44 percent of our
conversations (Pasupathi & Carstensen, 2003). That means internal
or external, conscious or unconscious self-esteem or social pressures can cause
us to distort our recollection and/or report of experiences. In either
case, the distortion impairs our ability to learn from experience and causes us
to repeat self-defeating habits.
My advice is for you to recruit a new form of the self to stand
beside the experiencing and remembering selves. I call this the
"monitoring self,” a self who does its best to faithfully record and
recall our experiences. The monitoring self knows that we want to forget
the uncomfortable features of our unhealthful experiences and that that
predilection can sabotage our change implementation. An effective
monitoring self knows that when we deliberately or inadvertently program out
from our insight or conversations information about our health lapses, we
reinforce false and self-defeating health-relevant narratives as well.
References
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. New
York: Macmillan.
Pasupathi, M., & Carstensen, L. L. (2003). Age and emotional experience
during mutual reminiscing. Psychology
and Aging, 18, 430–442.
No comments:
Post a Comment